Philosophy & Psychology

Animal consciousness: Time to rethink our human-centred approach

While we may enjoy the company of companion animals or a fleeting encounter with wildlife, many people believe humans have a superior consciousness of the world we live in.

Every now and then, though, new study findings about the surprising intelligence of other animals reignite this debate. Recently, two German philosophers, Professor Leonard Dung and PhD candidate Albert Newen, published a paper questioning whether we are coming at the issue from the right angle, or even asking the right question at all.

In their article, the authors say we should stop approaching animal consciousness as a ‘do they/don’t they?’ question. Rather, they suggest we should measure non-human consciousness on a spectrum alongside human consciousness.

In my research, I have explored whether we should stop trying to compare other animals with humans to gauge which ones are ‘worthy’ of better treatment. My work doesn’t oppose the study of animal consciousness; it simply asks people to reflect on the reasons we are asking these questions.

There may be other forms of consciousness we cannot understand. Non-human animals’ precise relationship to human consciousness doesn’t make them less important.

We still do not know what makes the difference between being alive and having consciousness. In humans, the definition of consciousness is vague and speculative. For example, the Glasgow coma scale measures the expectation that a patient will regain consciousness, rather than defining its presence or absence. Neurologists can’t agree on what part of the brain consciousness is generated in – yet we try to measure it in non-human animals.

Even within the animal rights movement, there is conflict between those who defend animals based on their similarity to humans (moral theorists), and those who claim non-human animals have a right to exist regardless of our view of them (abolitionists).

The problem is, both perspectives discuss our treatment of animals from a human perspective. In her book In Neither Man Nor Beast, abolitionist Carol J. Adams calls this the “arrogant eye” of anthropocentrism – the distortion of our understanding of the world into models suited for humans.

Of course, as humans we can only really look at the world from a human perspective. But anthropocentrism presumes there to be only one ‘objective’ perspective – the human one – and that Earth’s other organisms should measure up as close to humans as possible in order to be granted the right to exist. This implies that many non-human animals require no ethical consideration at all when it comes to their welfare.

A longstanding paradox is the status of animals used in research. They are similar enough to stand in for humans, yet a lot of people don’t want to think about what this means for their consciousness of pain and suffering. It seems an uncomfortable inconsistency.

Equally, many scientists working in artificial intelligence (AI)stem cell research and other fields are trying to reduce the exploitation of non-human animals in medical development – for example, the Dr Hadwen Trust, whose research does not involve testing on animals.

It’s important to understand our motives behind measuring animal consciousness. A lot of people seem to want to measure it to alleviate their guilt, by ‘othering’ the animals we harm from those we find appealing or similar to ourselves. Studying animal consciousness can help us empathise with non-human animals, but it can also help people avoid grappling with the ethics of animal testing.

I believe we need to stop asking questions about animal consciousness that are based on a hierarchy.

Octopi and other cephalopods have nervous systems throughout all their limbs. Their bodies are not a separate thing controlled by a brain or central nervous system.

My work doesn’t oppose the study of animal consciousness; it simply asks people to reflect on the reasons we are asking these questions.

So measuring consciousness using a central nervous system like ours may lead us to believe they do not have pain capacity or even sentience. Yet behavioural studies show they express both, just differently to humans.

Many animals express reproductive behaviour in ways completely alien to humans. For example, the female mole has an ovoteste and, outside of mating season, behaves like a male. (Mole ovotestes release eggs like typical ovaries but also have testicular tissue to one side that releases large amounts of male sex hormones.) Similarly, clown fish change from male to female, and kobudai fish change from female to male.

These species show how rich and diverse the animal kingdom is. Viewing them and other animals as “lesser” versions of ourselves denies the rich and complex diversity of the animal kingdom.

We are in an age which, to an extent, embraces feminism, anti-racism and anti-ableism. Perhaps it is also time to include ‘speciesism’ in our discussions about ethics – since valuing some species over others is a form of prejudice.

Over time, the public has slowly broadened its criticism of animal testing from great apes to baboons, mice and even water fleas. This shows we have placed animals in a hierarchy which makes experimenting on some acceptable and others less so. Philosophers have been raising concerns about the ethics of this since the sixth century BC.

This is also the age of the Anthropocene, the period during which human activities have affected the environment enough to create a distinct geological change. We live in a climate and nature crisis of our own making.

If we are serious about revolutionising our use of the Earth, it is time to rethink our need to classify all forms of life. We may find this is not about curiosity, but a desire to vindicate our history of dominion over the Earth. How about we exchange hierarchy for care? The future may depend upon it.

This article was originally published in The Conversation.

Image by Klara Kulikova on Unsplash.

author-avatar

About Patricia MacCormack

Patricia MacCormack is Professor of Continental Philosophy in the Faculty of Arts, Humanities and Social Sciences, Anglia Ruskin University, United Kingdom. She has published extensively in the areas of Continental philosophy, feminism, queer theory, posthuman ethics, animal studies and horror films.

One thought on “Animal consciousness: Time to rethink our human-centred approach

  1. Avatar photo Gary Bakker says:

    I started to write a Comment, but found I had problems/disagreement with every single one of Patricia’s first 10 paragraphs. Where to start?!
    My summary response is that we have had over 3,000 years of philosophical wordplay, and are still awaiting one consensually agreed answer, solution, or progression on any meaningful question, while Philosophy shrinks in the face of Science apace with ‘the God of the gaps’.
    In order to persist with questions about gods and consciousness and morality, Philosophy must loosen the definitions of such terms until they are so much woollier than the operational definitions of Science that they are in equal measure unanswerable and therefore meaningless.

Got a Comment?